Skip to content Skip to navigation

AREP Research Projects

More information

Learn more about the program on the Alumni Research Experience Program page

2014 Projects

"Who Would You Fund?" Study

Researcher: Lynn G. Chin, PhD  2012, Assistant Professor, Sociology, Washington and Lee University

This experimental study was designed to investigate whether evaluations of male and female leaders are differentially impacted by the structure of the organizations they lead. There were two ideal types of organizational structures in this experiment: hierarchical versus egalitarian. Respondents were probed to see whether stereotypes about organization structure interact with gender stereotypes to shape perceptions of men versus women's leadership abilities, regardless of leadership style.

AREP experimental data turned up mixed results.  I did not find any gender differences attribution of responsibility for corporate outcomes between male and female CEOs when directly asking respondents the degree to which they found CEO's responsible for company outcomes.  However, I did find that when egalitarian companies succeeded, the recommended pay increase for female CEOs was less than for male CEOs.

Social Beliefs and Consumer Preferences

Researcher: Elizabeth Hoberg, Postdoctoral Scholar, Sociology

In this study, we explored how relatively affluent people think and feel about their own wealth--as fair or unfair, deserved or undeserved--and whether those experiences predict their attitudes toward material possessions. As expected, we found considerable variation in people's experiences of their own wealth: Some reported feeling wealthier than they deserve to be whereas others indicated that their level of wealth is fair and well-deserved. Moreover, people who view their wealth as unfair and undeserved expressed less interest in owning designer label possessions, and were less likely to own luxury vehicles, compared to people who view their wealth as fair and deserved. These results could indicate that people who feel undeserving prefer to downplay or conceal their wealth from others whereas people who feel highly deserving prefer to signal their high socioeconomic status to others.

Related conference paper: Piff, P.K., Horberg, E.J., and Monin, B. (2015, February). An Embarrassment of Riches: Defining, Refining and Understanding the Novel Concept of "Wealth Guilt." Paper presented at the 16th Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality an Social Psychology, Long Beach, CA.

Employment and Housing Discrimination Study

Researchers: Shelley Correll, Cecilia Ridgeway & Molly King, PhD Candidate, Sociology

This experimental study seeks to assess the extent and comparative degree of discrimination in labor and housing markets. The Alumni REP pool provided valuable assistance with testing and validating the survey experiment design with a non-college-aged population. Our overarching hypotheses test the relative degree of discrimination between markets and demographic categories.

Publication of Results Pending

Motivational Sources of End of Life Care Planning

Researcher: Tamara Sims, PhD 2013, Postdoctoral Research Fellow, Psychology


In this two-part study, we sought to identify what motivates people to plan for end of life care. In the first part of our survey, we found that people strongly preferred an appeal to plan for end of life when it encouraged them to do so for their loved ones compared to other appeals that did not focus on socioemotional aspects of end of life planning. These findings indicate that end of life decisions are socially and emotionally meaningful rather than individualized and clinical.
 
In the second part of our study, we explored how making end of life more of a concrete reality affects people's motivation to think about and discuss their own death. To do so, we randomly assigned half of our participants to a control group where they viewed a current photo of themselves and the other half to a "concrete end of life" group where they viewed an age-progressed version of their photo. We found that across the life span, people seeing the age-progressed photo reported being less willing to think about and discuss death and dying. These findings suggest that when end of life becomes a reality, people may avoid planning for it.

Related conference paper: Sims, T., Carstensen, L.L., & Goldstein, M.K. "Health Status Moderates the Role of Future Time Perspective in End-of-life Planning. As part of Sims, T. & Nothoff, N. "May the Source be with you: Motivational Sources Underlying Older Adults' Optimization of Health and Well-being." Gerontological Society of America, Washington D.C., November 8, 2014.

2013 Projects

Using Attributions about Well-being To Relieve Stress

Researcher: Alexandra Gatherer Russell, PhD Candidate, Psychology

My research investigates the relationship between individuals’ beliefs and the coping styles that they endorse. Specifically, I investigated whether differences in implicit theories of well-being (or beliefs that well-being is malleable or fixed) are related to the coping strategies that individuals endorse. Literature on implicit theories in other domains shows that beliefs about the malleability of specific attributes have important consequences for relevant behaviors and outcomes (see, e.g., Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Dweck & Leggett, 1988). For instance, people who think that intelligence is malleable see academic difficulties and even failures as challenges, temporary setbacks, and opportunities for learning, while individuals who view intelligence as fixed view effort negatively, thinking it signifies low intelligence. As a result, these individuals respond poorly to failures and setbacks, withdrawing from schoolwork and getting worse grades in challenging subjects over time, while those with malleable views of intelligence respond adaptively to setbacks and do better in school over time (Blackwell et al., 2007; Dweck & Elliott-Moskwa, 2010; Dweck & Leggett, 1988).

This prior research on implicit theories in other domains led me to hypothesize that holding a malleable theory of psychological well-being would lead people to endorse and engage in more adaptive coping behaviors in the face of stress. I also expected that believing that well-being is malleable would make people more interested in learning about and improving their well-being and happiness. Prior to the study run using A-REP participants, I first ran a study that found that those with a more malleable conception of well-being are more likely to endorse adaptive responses to stressful scenarios. Next, I ran a study investigating whether those imparted with a more malleable conception of well-being are more likely to 1) endorse adaptive responding to stressful situations and 2) show interest in learning about well-being, stress and happiness, and I found that this was indeed the case. Using A-REP participants, my final study investigated whether those given a more malleable conception of well-being are more likely to take advantage of an opportunity to relieve situationally-induced stress with a mindfulness exercise. Study 3 also investigated whether those given a more malleable conception of well-being would be more likely to embrace goals and priorities related to happiness and well-being. As hypothesized, my results confirmed that those taught to have a more malleable conception of well-being were more likely to spend time on a mindfulness exercise after experiencing a mild stressor. These individuals were also more likely to embrace goals and priorities related to happiness and psychological well-being.

Cultural Consumption and Status

Researcher: Sandra Nakagawa, Ph.D Candidate, Sociology

We found that Stanford alumni rated first generation college applicants higher than continuing student applicants on several positive attributes such as intelligence, confidence, and sophistication. When asked whether the alumni would recommend a first generation college applicant or a continuing one, the alumni sample showed some preference for the first generation student (though this finding is only marginally significant by conventional statistical significance standards).

Research on cultural taste and status is ongoing within this project.

Health Promotion and Weight Discrimination

Researcher: Karen Powroznik, Ph.D Candidate, Sociology

Research results were presented at the Group Processes Pre-Conference at the 2013 American Sociological Association National Conference.

This research examines how neoliberal health beliefs direct, amplify, and justify expressions of prejudice and acts of discrimination.  Specifically, I investigate whether health promotion programs foster organizational cultures that legitimate the expression of anti-fat attitudes and contribute to increases in weight-based discrimination in employment decisions.

In this study I use an experimental design to examine whether fat employees are evaluated more negatively and receive lower recommendations for promotion when they are evaluated in a company that has an employee health promotion program.  Preliminary findings indicate that fat employees are less likely to receive recommendations for promotion than thin employees and are offered lower bonuses than thin employees. Findings from this study also suggest that fat employees are viewed as less competent and hardworking when health promotion is primed.  When employees are evaluated for the same company without an employee wellness program differences between fat and thin employees on such traits as intelligence, self-control, self-confidence, organization, laziness, and efficiency are not significant.

These findings suggest that health promotion beliefs motivate evaluators to use assumptions about body size and behavior to make judgments about the work ethic and abilities of others. By examining the beliefs and attitudes that generate and motivate weight-based discrimination this study provides a key insight into the findings of previous studies that document the pervasiveness of fat discrimination across a variety of domains. This research also contributes to the study of stigmatization, stereotypes, and discrimination processes within organizational settings and proposes new applications for the prejudice justification and suppression model.

Exploring Explanations for Singleness

Researchers: Corey Fields, Professor of Sociology, & Karen Powroznik, Ph.D Candidate, Sociology

Research results were presented at the 2013 Society for the Study of Social Problems Conference.

This study explores perceptions of singleness by examining differences in the explanations given for why an individual is single.  Using an online experiment we investigate how people’s explanations differ according to the race and gender of the single person. Our main finding is that men are thought to be more in control of their dating lives than women. Women’s singleness is believed to be determined by factors outside of a woman’s control and this gender difference is most pronounced for Black women. Beliefs about why a person is single influences how they are viewed by others and this has consequences for future success in the dating market.  We found that participants were least willing to set up the single white man, who was seen as most in control of his dating successes and failures.

2009 Projects

people sitting around meeting table

Selecting a Committee Member

Researcher:  Emily Zitek, PhD '10, Psychology, with Jessica Cameron and Valerie Jones Taylor

In this study, we were interested in how perceptions of a candidate for a Stanford committee leadership position depend on the candidate’s gender and the current makeup of the committee. Participants learned about the “Future Campus Committee,” and they were told that its purpose was to draft long-term goals for the campus, including construction and traffic management plans. Participants then read about the person being considered for the leader of this committee as well as the current gender composition of the committee (i.e., whether the committee had mostly men, mostly women, or an even split). The committee’s current gender composition did not affect participants’ perceptions of the candidate, but the candidate’s gender did. Participants slightly preferred the female candidate to the male candidate. For example, they thought the female candidate was more qualified than the male candidate, even though everything except the gender of the candidates was the same. We believe this result may demonstrate that people are now beginning to see the value of increasing the number of women in leadership roles.

screen shot of profile page

Dynamics in Peer-to-Peer Lending Markets

Researcher: Sarah Harkness, PhD Candidate '11, Sociology

One of the key ways individuals accumulate new wealth is first through the receipt of credit, yet the process of obtaining credit is potentially fraught with bias.  Researchers have examined various lending processes and have consistently documented group-level disparities in funding outcomes.  New forms of credit markets have been developed with the partial intent of mitigating these inequities.  They allow peers to finance each other without having to go through conventional lending institutions whose members may, perhaps inadvertently, use discriminatory underwriting methods.  Known as peer-to-peer lending markets, these online forums allow individuals to publicly display their lending requests and credit information in the hopes of finding lenders who will finance them.  In these markets, much as in more traditional lending scenarios, borrowers tend to display their personal information, which reveals many indicators on which bias and discrimination may be based.  In this project, I examine one type of indicator by assessing the extent to which status and trustworthiness systematically bias funding decisions while controlling for classic measures of creditworthiness.  I expect that lenders use status as an indicator of creditworthiness, in that they will entrust their resources to higher status individuals whom they assume have the competence to use the funds faithfully and repay the loan.

To assess implicit bias and discrimination in lending practices, participants evaluated a series of systematically varied loan applications using two different methodologies.  Both studies reveal that assessments of status and trustworthiness strongly influence all types of funding decisions.  Additionally, the results demonstrate the importance of examining the effects of status indicators, such as gender, race/ethnicity, and education, in consort as the combinations of these characteristics yield striking results.  For instance, while white males are highly advantaged in terms of their loan outcomes overall, they are strongly penalized for demonstrating a lack of educational development.  The intersections of race and gender also show that African American males continue to be highly disadvantaged in a wide variety of lending decisions, while the funding that African American females receive is similar to that of white males.  Overall, these results suggest that status indicators influence assessments of creditworthiness in complex ways, but, more generally, status and trustworthiness appear to be a source of systematic bias in lending. 

This study addresses one potential path through which preexisting status divisions and social inequalities perpetuate themselves by demonstrating that those who are advantaged by their characteristics in all levels of the socio-economic hierarchy will have their economic and social circumstances enhanced by virtue of their status distinctions, while lower status individuals will not be so privileged and may even be harmed.  By examining the decision-making processes behind lending, this project also helps illuminate how implicit bias and assessments of competence that vary by status groups may affect funding decisions in traditional credit markets more broadly.  Obtaining credit allows many individuals to eventually generate new wealth.  Yet, wealth accumulation is one of the predominant disparities of our time, thereby making the examination of the mechanisms underlying the receipt of credit so important.

business logo

Contemporary Issues in Business

Researcher:  Jamillah Bowman, JD/PhD Candidate '11, Law/Sociology

Clarifying the conditions under which growing organizational diversity is most likely to have positive rather than negative outcomes is a critical challenge of the 21st century. While valuing diversity has emerged as a popular buzzword for organizations, little research has investigated the social psychological impact of this movement.  This study examined how different types of cues regarding diversity influence management decisions, intergroup attitudes and beliefs about diversity.

Two diversity strategies were examined – a business case rationale for diversity and a legal rationale for diversity.  A business case emphasizes the profit and performance benefits of diversity, such as innovation, improved problem solving and addressing diverse market needs. A legal rationale emphasizes the need for diversity to comply with antidiscrimination laws and preventing litigation.  Despite the perceived legitimacy of profit based rationales endorsed by Corporate America, I found that a “business case” for diversity generated more resistance to inclusion than legal rationales that emphasized antidiscrimination law.  Participants exposed to antidiscrimination law were more likely to acknowledge institutional bias, express a value of diversity, and recommend a minority candidate for promotion than those exposed to a business case for diversity.

These results suggest that framing may influence the effectiveness of diversity efforts, including the ability to reduce inequality and promote social change.  Although the business case for diversity is a common rationale for inclusion, it may generate negative intergroup attitudes that restrict the positive outcomes that organizational leaders anticipate.  Beyond scientific merits, results can be used by leaders to better evaluate alternative policies, structural arrangements, and training strategies that seek to institutionalize the value of diversity. 

logos of stores like target and amazon

P&P Mall: Product Information Website Study

Researcher:  Dean Eckles, PhD Candidate ‘13, Communications

In this study we investigated how people's perceptions of what their attitudes are based on and their beliefs about what their attitudes ought to be based affect the certainty of those attitudes. In somewhat more technical language, we investigated the interactive effects of preferred and perceived attitude bases on attitude certainty. Participants evaluated a number of consumer products which were presented with the (ostensible) evaluations of these products by others (peer opinions). Of course, since little other product information was available, these peer opinions affected participants' attitudes towards the products. Likewise, participants who said that one ought not to use such majority opinions in determining one's own attitude were less influenced by these peer opinions. Following this initial product evaluation, participants were given random feedback about whether their attitudes were based on the consensus information. When participants were told they likely based their attitude on consensus information but also think one ought not to do so, their attitude certainty was reduced following the feedback.

This study was a first investigation into such metacognition -- thinking about one's own thinking process -- in persuasion.   This work is part of a larger investigation into individual differences in persuasion. Our previous work has demonstrated large individual differences in how people respond to particular influence strategies. This research helps to explain why some of these observed differences exist. We hope to help push psychological research towards better accounting for variation among people. Applications include commerce, political communication, coaching, and behavior-change support.

Race as a Determinate of Just Criminal Punishment

Researcher: Beth  Red Bird, PhD Candidate '11, Sociology

The policy discussion of criminal sentencing is rife with controversy. The result of such debate is a cornucopia of sentencing legislation that allows judges varying degrees of freedom. Sentencing scheme types include: simple, determinate sentences (exact number of years in prison), indeterminate or open-ended sentences (e.g., 5 to 15, 20 to life), and options for probation (instead of incarceration) or parole (post-incarceration supervision). Judges are responsible for decisions on whether to incarcerate, how long to incarcerate, and whether to require parole.

Supporters of judicial discretion argue that legislative guidelines require stricter sentences for crimes more likely to be committed by black defendants. The most commonly cited of these is the gross disparity between sentences for similar amounts of crack cocaine (with its significant presence in the black community) and powder cocaine (largely used by whites). Conversely, proponents of sentencing schemes argue that judges sentencing with no legislative structure impose almost universally harsher sentences for non-white defendants. A large number of studies have found that race is significantly correlated to length of sentence1. Given sentence disparities identified by prior research, the question is: how do sentencing schemes affect the fairness of sentences?

This project utilized the experimental method to isolate the causal effect that socioeconomic and demographic factors have on assessments of a just sentence at four levels: (1) decision on length of incarceration within determinate sentencing structures; (2) decision on length of incarceration within indeterminate sentences; (3) decision to increase sentence due to special circumstances, and; (4) decision to decrease sentence due to special circumstances.

Results show that black defendants are more likely than whites to be given longer sentences.  In addition, when respondents were asked to evaluate the defendant’s likelihood of reoffense, blacks were considered more likely to reoffend and were correspondingly more likely to receive an increased sentence.  However, when respondents were asked to evaluate the defendant’s likelihood of successful reintegration, black defendants were more likely to receive a reduced sentence.  Overall, the results indicate that blacks are more likely than whites to be sentenced at the extremes of the judicial sentencing range.

Researcher:  Gaurav Sood, PhD Candidate ‘11, Political Science